- How to Think Like Sherlock Holmes -
MARIA KONNIKOVA
Published in Great Britain in 2013 by Canongate Books Ltd,
14 High Street, Edinburgh EH1 1TE
This digital edition first published in 2013 by Canongate Books Copyright © Maria Konnikova, 2013
The moral right of the author has been asserted Portions of this book appeared in a different form on the website Big Think (www.bigthink.com) and in Scientific American
Book Details
Price
|
3.00 |
---|---|
Pages
| 256 p |
File Size
|
1,636 KB |
File Type
|
PDF format |
eISBN
| 978 0 85786 726 1 |
Copyright©
| Maria Konnikova, 2013 |
When I was little, my dad used to read us Sherlock Holmes stories before bed.
While my brother often took the opportunity to fall promptly asleep on his
corner of the couch, the rest of us listened intently. I remember the big leather
armchair where my dad sat, holding the book out in front of him with one arm,
the dancing flames from the fireplace reflecting in his black-framed glasses. I
remember the rise and fall of his voice as the suspense mounted beyond all
breaking points, and finally, finally, at long last the awaited solution, when it all
made sense and I’d shake my head, just like Dr. Watson, and think, Of course;
it’s all so simple now that he says it. I remember the smell of the pipe that my
dad himself would smoke every so often, a fruity, earthy mix that made its way
into the folds of the leather chair, and the outlines of the night through the
curtained French windows. His pipe, of course, was ever-so-slightly curved just
like Holmes’s. And I remember that final slam of the book, the thick pages
coming together between the crimson covers, when he’d announce, “That’s it for
tonight.” And off we’d go—no matter how much begging and pleading we’d try
and what sad faces we’d make—upstairs, up to bed.
And then there’s the one thing that wedged its way so deeply into my brain
that it remained there, taunting me, for years to come, when the rest of the stories
had long since faded into some indeterminate background and the adventures of
Holmes and his faithful Boswell were all but forgotten: the steps.
The steps to 221B Baker Street. How many were there? It’s the question
Holmes brought before Watson in “A Scandal in Bohemia,” and a question that
never once since left my mind. As Holmes and Watson sit in their matching
armchairs, the detective instructs the doctor on the difference between seeing
and observing. Watson is baffled. And then, all at once everything becomes crystal clear.
“When I hear you give your reasons,” [Watson] remarked, “the thing always
appears to me to be so ridiculously simple that I could easily do it myself,
though at each successive instance of your reasoning, I am baffled until you
explain your process. And yet I believe that my eyes are as good as yours.”
“Quite so,” [Holmes] answered, lighting a cigarette, and throwing himself
down into an armchair. “You see, but you do not observe. The distinction is
clear. For example, you have frequently seen the steps which lead up from the
hall to this room.”
“Frequently.”
“How often?”
“Well, some hundreds of times.”
“Then how many are there?”
“How many? I don’t know.”
“Quite so! You have not observed. And yet you have seen. That is just my
point. Now, I know that there are seventeen steps, because I have both seen and observed.”
When I first heard it, on one firelit, pipe-smoke-filled evening, the exchange
shook me. Feverishly, I tried to remember how many steps there were in our own
house (I had not the faintest idea), how many led up to our front door (I drew a
beautiful blank), how many led down to the basement (ten? twenty? I couldn’t
even approximate). And for a long time afterward, I tried to count stairs and
steps whenever I could, lodging the proper number in my memory in case
anyone ever called upon me to report. I’d make Holmes proud.
Of course, I’d promptly forget each number I so diligently tried to remember
—and it wasn’t until later that I realized that by focusing so intently on
memorization, I’d missed the point entirely. My efforts had been doomed from the start.
What I couldn’t understand then was that Holmes had quite a bit more than a
leg up on me. For most of his life, he had been honing a method of mindful
interaction with the world. The Baker Street steps? Just a way of showing off a
skill that now came so naturally to him that it didn’t require the least bit of
thought. A by-the-way manifestation of a process that was habitually, almost
subconsciously, unfolding in his constantly active mind. A trick, if you will, of
no real consequence, and yet with the most profound implications if you stopped
to consider what made it possible. A trick that inspired me to write an entire
book in its honor.
The idea of mindfulness itself is by no means a new one. As early as the end of
the nineteenth century, William James, the father of modern psychology, wrote
that “the faculty of voluntarily bringing back a wandering attention, over and
over again, is the very root of judgment, character, and will. . . . An education
which should improve this faculty would be the education par excellence.” That
faculty, at its core, is the very essence of mindfulness. And the education that
James proposes, an education in a mindful approach to life and to thought.
In the 1970s, Ellen Langer demonstrated that mindfulness could reach even
further than improving “judgment, character, and will.” A mindful approach
could go as far as to make elderly adults feel and act younger—and could even
improve their vital signs, such as blood pressure, and their cognitive function. In
recent years, studies have shown that meditation-like thought (an exercise in the
very attentional control that forms the center of mindfulness), for as little as
fifteen minutes a day, can shift frontal brain activity toward a pattern that has
been associated with more positive and more approach-oriented emotional states,
and that looking at scenes of nature, for even a short while, can help us become
more insightful, more creative, and more productive. We also know, more
definitively than we ever have, that our brains are not built for multitasking—
something that precludes mindfulness altogether. When we are forced to do
multiple things at once, not only do we perform worse on all of them but our
memory decreases and our general well-being suffers a palpable hit.
But for Sherlock Holmes, mindful presence is just a first step. It’s a means to a
far larger, far more practical and practically gratifying goal. Holmes provides
precisely what William James had prescribed: an education in improving our
faculty of mindful thought and in using it in order to accomplish more, think
better, and decide more optimally. In its broadest application, it is a means for
improving overall decision making and judgment ability, starting from the most
basic building block of your own mind.
What Homes is really telling Watson when he contrasts seeing and observing
is to never mistake mindlessness for mindfulness, a passive approach with an
active involvement. We see automatically: a stream of sensory inputs that
requires no effort on our part, save that of opening our eyes. And we see
unthinkingly, absorbing countless elements from the world without necessarily
processing what those elements might be. We may not even realize we’ve seen
something that was right before our eyes. But when we observe, we are forced to
pay attention. We have to move from passive absorption to active awareness. We
have to engage. It’s true for everything—not just sight, but each sense, each
input, each thought.
All too often, when it comes to our own minds, we are surprisingly mindless.
We sail on, blithely unaware of how much we are missing, of how little we grasp
of our own thought process—and how much better we could be if only we’d
taken the time to understand and to reflect. Like Watson, we plod along the same
staircase tens, hundreds, thousands of times, multiple times a day, and we can’t
begin to recall the most mundane of details about them (I wouldn’t be surprised
if Holmes had asked about color instead of number of steps and had found
Watson equally ignorant).
But it’s not that we aren’t capable of doing it; it’s just that we don’t choose to
do it. Think back to your childhood. Chances are, if I asked you to tell me about
the street where you grew up, you’d be able to recall any number of details. The
colors of the houses. The quirks of the neighbors. The smells of the seasons.
How different the street was at different times of day. Where you played. Where
you walked. Where you were afraid of walking. I bet you could go on for hours.
As children, we are remarkably aware. We absorb and process information at
a speed that we’ll never again come close to achieving. New sights, new sounds,
new smells, new people, new emotions, new experiences: we are learning about
our world and its possibilities. Everything is new, everything is exciting,
everything engenders curiosity. And because of the inherent newness of our
surroundings, we are exquisitely alert; we are absorbed; we take it all in. And
what’s more, we remember: because we are motivated and engaged (two
qualities we’ll return to repeatedly), we not only take the world in more fully
than we are ever likely to do again, but we store it for the future. Who knows
when it might come in handy?
But as we grow older, the blasé factor increases exponentially. Been there,
done that, don’t need to pay attention to this, and when in the world will I ever
need to know or use that? Before we know it, we have shed that innate
attentiveness, engagement, and curiosity for a host of passive, mindless habits.
And even when we want to engage, we no longer have that childhood luxury.
Gone are the days where our main job was to learn, to absorb, to interact; we
now have other, more pressing (or so we think) responsibilities to attend to and
demands on our minds to address. And as the demands on our attention increase
—an all too real concern as the pressures of multitasking grow in the
increasingly 24/7 digital age—so, too, does our actual attention decrease. As it
does so, we become less and less able to know or notice our own thought habits,
and more and more allow our minds to dictate our judgments and decisions,
instead of the other way around. And while that’s not inherently a bad thing—in
fact, we’ll be talking repeatedly about the need to automate certain processes
that are at first difficult and cognitively costly—it is dangerously close to
mindlessness. It’s a fine line between efficiency and thoughtlessness—and one
that we need to take care not to cross.
You’ve likely had the experience where you need to deviate from a stable
routine only to find that you’ve somehow forgotten to do so. Let’s say you need
to stop by the drugstore on your way home. All day long, you remember your
errand. You rehearse it; you even picture the extra turn you’ll have to take to get
there, just a quick step from your usual route. And yet somehow, you find
yourself back at your front door, without having ever stopped off. You’ve
forgotten to take that turn and you don’t even remember passing it. It’s the habit
mindlessly taking over, the routine asserting itself against whatever part of your
mind knew that it needed to do something else.
It happens all the time. You get so set in a specific pattern that you go through
entire chunks of your day in a mindless daze (and if you are still thinking about
work? worrying about an email? planning ahead for dinner? forget it). And that
automatic forgetfulness, that ascendancy of routine and the ease with which a
thought can be distracted, is just the smallest part—albeit a particularly
noticeable one, because we have the luxury of realizing that we’ve forgotten to
do something—of a much larger phenomenon. It happens much more regularly
than we can point to—and more often than not, we aren’t even aware of our own
mindlessness. How many thoughts float in and out of your head without your
stopping to identify them? How many ideas and insights have escaped because
you forgot to pay attention? How many decisions or judgments have you made
without realizing how or why you made them, driven by some internal default
settings of whose existence you’re only vaguely, if at all, aware? How many
days have gone by where you suddenly wonder what exactly you did and how
you got to where you are?
This book aims to help. It takes Holmes’s methodology to explore and explain
the steps necessary for building up habits of thought that will allow you to
engage mindfully with yourself and your world as a matter of course. So that
you, too, can offhandedly mention that number of steps to dazzle a less-with-it companion.
So, light that fire, curl up on that couch, and prepare once more to join
Sherlock Holmes and Dr. John H. Watson on their adventures through the crime
filled streets of London—and into the deepest crevices of the human mind.
Introduction
Table of Contents
Prelude
PART ONE
UNDERSTANDING (YOURSELF)
CHAPTER ONE
The Scientific Method of the Mind
CHAPTER TWO
The Brain Attic: What Is It and What’s in There?
PART TWO
FROM OBSERVATION TO IMAGINATION
CHAPTER THREE
Stocking the Brain Attic: The Power of Observation
CHAPTER FOUR
Exploring the Brain Attic: The Value of Creativity and Imagination
PART THREE
THE ART OF DEDUCTION
CHAPTER FIVE
Navigating the Brain Attic: Deduction from the Facts
CHAPTER SIX
Maintaining the Brain Attic: Education Never Stops
PART FOUR
THE SCIENCE AND ART OF SELF-KNOWLEDGE
CHAPTER SEVEN
The Dynamic Attic: Putting It All Together
CHAPTER EIGHT
We’re Only Human
Postlude
Acknowledgments
Further Reading
Index
Choice of attention—to pay attention to this and ignore that—is to the inner life
what choice of action is to the outer. In both cases man is responsible for his
choice and must accept the consequences. As Ortega y Gasset said: “Tell me to
what you pay attention, and I will tell you who you are.”
—W. H. AUDEN